Children Aren’t Manipulating—They’re Communicating by Kathy Dane
“When we’re children, we communicate like musicians who are still learning to play their instruments.” — Brad Montague
Young children are often misunderstood when their behavior is labeled as manipulative or defiant. But neuroscience and developmental psychology tell us a different story: children are not trying to control adults—they’re trying to communicate unmet needs in the only way their developing brains allow.
The prefrontal cortex—the part of the brain responsible for impulse control, planning, and emotional regulation—is one of the last areas to fully develop. In fact, it continues maturing well into a person’s mid-twenties (Casey et al., 2005). This means that young children often operate from more reactive, emotional brain regions (like the amygdala), rather than intentionally calculated decision-making.
Developmentally, children do not yet have the language skills or self-awareness to express complex internal experiences. Instead, they "speak" through behavior. Montague’s quote captures this beautifully: children are still learning how to use the instruments of communication and self-regulation. Tantrums, refusals, or tears are not signs of manipulation but attempts to be heard.
Language plays a powerful role in this process. Research shows that the words children hear from caregivers become the foundation of their inner voice—a process that begins with private speech (talking aloud to oneself) and gradually becomes internalized as self-talk (Winsler et al., 2009). Over time, the way we speak to children shapes how they speak to themselves.
This makes the adult’s choice of language critical. When we respond with empathy, curiosity, and clear boundaries, we help build a child’s brain architecture in ways that support long-term emotional regulation and resilience. In contrast, when we label behavior as manipulative or bad, we risk planting seeds of shame that can become part of a child’s self-concept.
Ultimately, children do not need correction nearly as much as they need connection. When we view behavior through the lens of development and need, we can respond not with frustration—but with compassion.
Kathleen Dane, MS, LPC Associate utilizes play therapy with child clients at Playroom Lubbock. She’s under the clinical supervision of Kelly Martin, MEd, LPC-S, RPT-S
References
Casey, B. J., Tottenham, N., Liston, C., & Durston, S. (2005). Imaging the developing brain: What have we learned about cognitive development? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(3), 104–110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.01.011
Center on the Developing Child at Harvard University. (2007). The science of early childhood development. https://developingchild.harvard.edu
Day, A., & Smith, C. (2013). Understanding the role of private speech in children's emotion regulation. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28(2), 405–414. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2012.12.001
Montague, B. (2019). Becoming better grownups: Rediscovering what matters and remembering how to fly. Avery.
Moriguchi, Y., & Hiraki, K. (2013). Prefrontal cortex and executive function in young children: A review of NIRS studies. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 867. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00867
Whedon, M., Holochwost, S. J., & Winsler, A. (2021). Private speech and the development of self-regulation: The importance of temperamental anger. Infant and Child Development, 30(6), e2293. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2293
Winsler, A., Fernyhough, C., & Montero, I. (2009). Private speech, executive functioning, and the development of verbal self-regulation. Cambridge University Press